Classroom observations - behavior of children with and without ADHD

The relationship between reaction time variability and observed attention in children with and without ADHD.

Published on

Tue 13 Aug. 2013

Classroom observations - behavior of children with and without ADHD

The relationship between reaction time variability and observed attention in children with and without ADHD

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) has become a fairly common subject of research in today’s society. The disorder affects working memory, attention span, and inhibitory control. There are two types, Combined and Predominantly Inattentive. In this study, 146 participants between the ages of 7 and 11 were split into three groups: ADHD-Combined (ADHD-C), ADHD-Predominantly Inattentive (ADHD-I), and a control. Tanya Antonini and colleagues looked for an association between reaction time (RT) variability and observed behavioral indicators of attention.

Building on results

Antonini et al.’s study (2013) built on the results that were presented in Epstein et al.’s study (2011). This study examined differences in RT variability between children with and without ADHD across five computerized tasks. The results of this study showed that the RTs of children with ADHD were more variable than those of the control group, and were also less accurate than the control on the majority of the tasks. This study did not have an analog math task.

Groupings

Both ADHD groups in Antonini et al.’s study had lower IQ and Numerical Operations scores and a higher number of inattentive symptoms than the control. If six or more symptoms of ADHD were present in the child, they fell into one of the categories (ADHD-I if symptoms were in the inattentive domain and ADHD-C if in both the inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive domains). To be in either ADHD group, the child must have fulfilled DSM-IV criteria.

The experiment

Children in the three groups completed five computerized neuropsychological tasks and one analog math task. The five computerized tasks that the participants took were designed to assess things like working memory, attention span, and inhibitory control. These tasks were the choice discrimination task, Attentional Network Task, go/no-go task, stop signal task, and n-back task. Though these tasks differed, they all required the participant to focus and push a button in certain events. For each neuropsychological task, five neuropsychological variables were calculated. During the analog test, behavior of the children was observed continuously using The Observer XT. Researchers looked at the amount of time the children spent on-task and off-task. The participant was considered off-task if their gaze left the sheet for two or more seconds. To measure attention, researchers calculated the mean duration of on-task behavior. Linear mixed models were then used to examine relationships between the RT variability measures from the neuropsychological tasks and the behavioral inattention observed during the analog task.








FREE TRIAL: Try The Observer XT yourself!

Request a free trial and see for yourself how easy behavioral research can be!

  • Work faster
  • Reduce costs
  • Get better data

The results

For both the ADHD-C and ADHD-I groups, the mean on-task behavior was significantly shorter than that of the control group. The mean did not vary significantly between the two groups with ADHD. There did appear to be a relationship between observed attention and RT variability, which spanned all of the groups in the study, staying consistent regardless of diagnosis. The relationship did not appear to be affected by age, gender, or presence of anxiety or depression. It is a strong relationship, as the associations were viewed across the five different tasks. There was also a significant positive relationship between task accuracy and mean on-task behavior, probably due to the fact that participants are only able to achieve high accuracy if they are paying attention.

In the future

Future studies should try to assess ADHD-related behaviors outside a lab setting. They should also consider supporting their coding of behavior with psychophysiological indicators of attention to better determine where the attention of the participants is directed.

  • Antonini, T.N.; Narad, M. E.; Langberg, J.M.; Epstein, J.N. (2013). Behavioral correlates of reaction time variability in children with and without ADHD. Neuropsychology27 (2), 201-209.
  • Epstein, J.N.; Langberg, J.M.; Rosen, P.J.; Graham, A.; Narad, M.E.; Antonini, T.N.; Brinkman, W.B.; Froehlich, T.; Simon, J.O.; Altaye, M. (2011). Evidence for higher reaction time variability for children with ADHD on a range of cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology, 25 (4), 427-441.

Related Posts

Observing effective leadership and teamwork
05 Dec human behavior research Psychology

Observing effective leadership and teamwork

How do organizations, leaders, teams, and employees work together to ensure efficient improvements and effective changes? The OBCC team combined video-based behavioral observations and physiological measures to examine this.
Studying conflict interactions between mothers and adolescents
05 Dec human behavior research Psychology

Studying conflict interactions between mothers and adolescents

It is common for conflicts to arise between adolescents and their parents. However, severe conflicts can have negative effects on adolescent development. What can parents do to prevent escalating conflicts?
What can you use eye tracking for?
20 Oct human behavior research Psychology

What can you use eye tracking for?

Eye tracking is a technique which records what you are looking at. With more complex analysis, it can give all sorts of information about a subject's mental state.